![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
![]() by Staff Writers Seoul, Korea (SPX) May 20, 2016
Scientists have understood quite well why so many poisonous animals have brightly colored bodies - the colors send a message to the predators: " don't eat me, or you'll get sick and die". But why some toxic animals actually hide the warning colors from the predator's view, showing them only at the very last moment, when they are already attacked? How can a sudden display of bright underwings at the moment of capture help a distasteful insect, such like the spotted lanternfly (see Figure and movie clip). In the recent issue of Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution a Korean-Polish team of behavioral ecologists have proposed a new explanation. Hyun-Joon Cho and Changku Kang, then graduate students at Seoul National University, spent hours preparing artificial prey models and presenting them to wild oriental tits (Parus minor). The birds were presented with hand-made bitter prey models that looked rather dull, imitating distasteful cryptic insects on the typical natural background such like the spotted lantern flies resting on the tree bark (left figure). These bitter prey models were mixed with tasty models that were also dull but differently colored. At the beginning, the birds picked up the bitter prey and dropped them right after grabbing it. But after several days of the same experiments, the birds slowly learned to use the colors to distinguish the bitter prey from the tasty prey, and they finally learned not to pick up the bitter prey. Some of the bitter prey models were cleverly designed such that the prey suddenly displayed conspicuous colour immediately after the bird grabbed the prey. This imitated the situation when the lanternfly opens its wings and presents red-white-black pattern on their hind wings in response to being attacked. This sudden display of conspicuous color dramatically speeded-up the learning process of birds. Only after a few experiences with the bitter inconspicuous prey with sudden display, the birds were able to distinguish these prey from other inconspicuous but tasty prey. Very quickly birds stopped picking up the inconspicuous bitter prey as if the sudden display made the learning much easier. We learn faster and remember better those events that were associated with something unexpected, something noticeable. It seems that the same happens to birds, and that some distasteful insects exploit this by using sudden displays. Because of the sudden displays, the predators quickly learn to discriminate the distasteful insects and those insects survive the encounters with predators. "This strategy helps the distasteful prey in two ways" says Kang. "The predators that are sensitive to the toxins produced by the prey can quickly learn to avoid the prey. But, those rare predators that are not sensitive to the toxins have difficulty spotting the prey because if its relatively cryptic appearance". Either way the prey wins. "The Laboratory of Behavioral Ecology and Evolution, together with collaborators from other disciplines at Seoul National University, is focusing now at the spotted lanternflies' display behaviour that helps these insects to avoid predation", comment Piotr Jablonski and Sang-Im Lee, the co-authors of the study.
Related Links Laboratory of Behavioral Ecology and Evolution at Seoul National University Darwin Today At TerraDaily.com
|
|
The content herein, unless otherwise known to be public domain, are Copyright 1995-2024 - Space Media Network. All websites are published in Australia and are solely subject to Australian law and governed by Fair Use principals for news reporting and research purposes. AFP, UPI and IANS news wire stories are copyright Agence France-Presse, United Press International and Indo-Asia News Service. ESA news reports are copyright European Space Agency. All NASA sourced material is public domain. Additional copyrights may apply in whole or part to other bona fide parties. All articles labeled "by Staff Writers" include reports supplied to Space Media Network by industry news wires, PR agencies, corporate press officers and the like. Such articles are individually curated and edited by Space Media Network staff on the basis of the report's information value to our industry and professional readership. Advertising does not imply endorsement, agreement or approval of any opinions, statements or information provided by Space Media Network on any Web page published or hosted by Space Media Network. General Data Protection Regulation (GDPR) Statement Our advertisers use various cookies and the like to deliver the best ad banner available at one time. All network advertising suppliers have GDPR policies (Legitimate Interest) that conform with EU regulations for data collection. By using our websites you consent to cookie based advertising. If you do not agree with this then you must stop using the websites from May 25, 2018. Privacy Statement. Additional information can be found here at About Us. |